As a trained biologist, I have often wondered where the persistent
myth that inbreeding is good for domestic breeds has come from.
This is particularly troublesome in light of the many examples found
in the wild suggesting that in nature inbreeding leads to inbreeding
depression, and ultimately to extinction (Frankham and Ralls, 1998).
Therefore, inbreeding is a serious threat for conservation biology
and a real problem where culture meets and splits natural breeding
grounds into smaller and smaller parcels. Consequently, zoos that
engage in captive breeding programs monitor very closely the degree
of inbreeding. In addition, nature has invented schemes to avoid
inbreeding via many different mechanisms. One is known as optimal
discrepancy, where most mating happens between distantly related
animals. Flowering plans have evolved molecular mechanisms to avoid
self-fertilization (Stone et al., 1999). Even the best known case
of high levels of naturally occurring inbreeding, the blind mole
rat, has genetic exchange between the different underground colonies,
thus keeping a degree of genetic diversity not unlike humans and
domestic cats (Page and Holmes, 1998).
So, why do we think that breeding dogs or other domestic animals
is any different? While I can not completely resolve this problem,
I found a few reasons that make it likely why this myth came into
existence. One of these reasons relates to the apparent achievement
of fixing a specific type that is so easily achievable by inbreeding.
Any natural breeding will result in some variation around a given
type. In contrast, even inbreeding over only a few generations will
result in reduced variability, thus making the offspring look more
alike because of loss of genes responsible for the increased variation.
We accept the price tag that comes with this in our food plants
and have to defend them against invasion by pathogens which would
wrack havoc in these diversity depleted populations if left alone
(Stachowicz et al., 1999). Similarly, many dog breeds will not survive
in the wild as we have selected for specific features and not necessary
for their ability to survive.
Thus, while inbreeding is a powerful tool to minimize variation
in a desired type, a breeder needs to be aware of the unwanted effects
of inbreeding that will ultimately result in the extinction of this
extremely homogenous line so carefully generated by inbreeding.
This are at least the lessons taught to us by nature. So, with this
knowledge in hand one wonders what other arguments are there in
favor of inbreeding, other then the desire to generate over the
short run dogs that closely fit the standard of the breed. One argument
I frequently hear is that inbreeding is actually beneficial for
a breed as it will unmask deleterious genes and can help eliminate
those carriers. I will show below that this is hardly a reasonable
argument. However, as with many bad science examples, once invented
they can hardly be eliminated because somebody will find the original
paper and simply be ignorant about all the contrary data and re-emphasize
the original idea. After all, we lived for several hundreds of years
with the flat earth myth.
As a biologist I am afraid to admit that apparently the idea of
inbreeding as being beneficial for reproduction apparently goes
back to C. Darwin. Darwin noted, as have others before him that
inbreeding does not appear to be a major mode of reproduction in
wild populations. However, he also noted that inbreeding in domestic
and wild populations causes inbreeding depression. Astonishingly,
one of his lines of morning glory flowers he studied for this inbreeding
depression phenomenon appeared to come out of this inbreeding depression
during Darwin's life time and appeared more healthy than lines of
limited outcrosses. Darwin named this line Hero, for obvious reasons.
From this example, Darwin and others concluded that somehow the
accumulation of bad inherited material can be purged and after an
inbreeding depression of variable length the population may emerge
healthy again or even healthier than the original population.
While apparently such purging and exit from inbreeding depression
can occur, Darwin had no idea how frequently that is or whether
it will regularly occur. Over the last 150 years we have learned
that the successful exit out of an inbreeding depression is in fact
very rare. Out of 52 published studies conducted, only two have
clearly shown that this can occur (Pennisi, 1999). All other cases
showed a lasting inbreeding depression with no apparent signs of
recovery or even extinction of populations. Moreover, recent attempts
to restore fertility in severely inbred populations by introducing
new individuals showed dramatic restoration of fertility and recovery
of local populations from the brink of extinction. So, what can
we learn from these examples for dog breeding? Apparently, firstly
we have to look at fertility rates of more inbred as compared to
outcrossed dog populations of various breeds. If statistics were
to be trusted and claims of fathers confirmed one would likely see
that multi generation of inbreeding will result in statistically
significantly lowered numbers of viable offspring. The recent public
exhibit of the cross of two mutts resulting in 17 puppies certainly
supports the notion of hybrid vigor at the offspring level. In humans,
inbreeding depression causes a 40% lethality or severe disabilities
of offspring produced from brother/sister mating (Page and Holmes,
1998) and it is a reasonable assumption that this will be comparable
in dog brother/sister mating.
The next question is, of course, what is the scientific basis for
inbreeding depression and the occasional success of purging the
genes responsible for this depression as well as exiting the inbreeding
depression as an apparently purified population. To be honest, nobody
knows for sure. This simply relates to the large number of still
unknown genes even in the human genome and the even less known interindividual
genetic variability. Thus it can well be that the two cases known
in which purging seems to have worked, may have started with less
genetic defects than those in which inbreeding led to extinction.
Can we know beforehand whether the population we want to breed falls
into one or the other category? Unfortunately not! If we would know
that, we could redesign our breeding efforts of endangered species.
As far as rare breeds of dogs are concerned, it is apparently bad
advice to bet on the occasional self-healing capacity of inbreeding.
More likely is that the dog breed in question will fall into the
category of disastrous outcome of inbreeding. This conclusion is
supported by a number of clinically relevant findings in inbred
dogs that would have been impossible to achieve in the less inbred
human population.
One outcome of inbreeding is that genes, which we inherit from
both father and mother as two slightly different variations, will
be more uniform. Less genetic variation makes animals look more
alike (and thus make them conform better to a given standard) but
also makes them more sensitive to spread of infections (more difficult
in a more heterogeneous population of hosts for a disease; Stachowicz
et al., 1999). The good part is, if one of these genes is defective
and causes a lethal mutation, the carrier will disappear in the
next generation. Thus, some people actuall think they are able to
purify through this approach their line. However, there are a number
of issues this assumption has not resolved. First, given that any
breeder will no be able to breed more than about 25 litters of multi
generations of inbreeding of sister/brother mating (assuming a mean
breeding age of two years for the dogs and a breedering program
of 50 years) any breeder will hardly be able to see the outcome
of his effort (either positive or negative). If we look for longer
breeding programs there is a population of lions in India which
have gone through almost 100 years (or about 50 generations) of
inbreeding. This population now has the lowest known genetic variation
of all wild animals tested to date (Page and Homes, 1998). While
still healthy, it is possible that any infection entering this population
will spread rapidly and erase the entire population. In contrast
to these lions, dogs have been domesticated for thousands of years
and have been selected to a reasonable extent to serve the whims
of their breeders, which are not necessarily compatible with a dogs
ability to sustain its life, a simple fact that rules the survival
of the lions mentioned above.
Another issue relates to the fact that the differences between
the father's and the mother's genetic material tends to be increased
by mutations every generation. This counteracts to some extent the
uniformity generated by inbreeding. In dog breeds which started
with small foundation populations and have been enlarged to several
thousand individuals in part by excessive inbreeding to fix the
type, this issue becomes a big problem simply because the selection
pressure applied (conformation to a specific type) does not take
all genes that are necessary to develop an animal into account.
Thus, while focusing on the perhaps 1000 genes relevant for the
desired traits, those breeders (and others before and after them)
have ignored the remaining 139,000 genes necessary for a fully functional
dog.
One of these dog breeds that was recently generated from a small
foundation population is the Doberman Pinscher. These dogs have
recently featured in a significant scientific discovery because
of their highly inbred background. The discovery is that a single
gene causes, if mutated, a condition called narcolepsy (Lin et al.,
1999). This condition is typically elicited by strong positive emotions.
The dog will jump up, all excited and suddenly collapse and fall
asleep. Because of the highly inbred strains of both Doberman Pinscher
and Labrador retrievers available, geneticists could isolate the
gene involved in this disease, and could characterize how this gene
causes this disease (Lin et al., 1999). However, while this is scientifically
useful, it does not help the breed. Clearly, in the wild an animal
that will fall asleep when it sees a mate or prey will not survive
as an individual nor propagate into the next generation. Nevertheless,
purging by inbreeding would likely not help as the carriers are
normal and show no symptoms. This is in contrast to other inherited
diseases such as human sickle cell anemia. While individuals carrying
two mutated genes are not viable, the carrier of a single mutated
gene is only impaired in his oxygen transport, but otherwise healthy.
Clearly, if a gene does not cause any recognizable phenotype in
the heterozygotic state it can not be selected against, and the
lethal homozygotic state will appear only in highly inbred population
in a few individuals (about 25% of each litter).
I often hear the argument that one should keep a genetically affected
animal for test breeding with presumed carriers. The logic is compelling,
so it seems. Once a carrier has been identified, it will not be
used for breeding. Good. But how about the siblings of the carrier?
Do we cull them all?? And how many test breeding do we need before
we can go ahead and breed that dog? About 25 to make sure that the
dog does not carry the most frequent genetic diseases? And how about
the less-frequent ones, and those that are not yet characterized
as being inherited? 25 times 4 puppies would mean 100 puppies have
been produced (and killed) just to make sure that the dog in question
does not carry any of the 25 mutated genes involved in the arbitrarily
defined 25 investigated genetic diseases. This does not appear to
be a humane and efficient way of approaching the problem.
Last, but not least, in order to do the test breeding you have
to have a dog that has the mutated genes. So, in order to test for
the 25 genetic diseases, you have to have the 25 sick dogs you need
for test breeding. Imagine anyone visiting a kennel to choose a
puppy and the breeder shows off with all the sick dogs they have
to do the numerous test breeding to generate a genetically healthy
(for the tested genes at least) dog. Again, the problems with the
test breeding and culling scenario are obvious.
In summary, in most dog breeds, and in particular in rare breeds,
inbreeding is not a solution but a problem. In fact, the very reason
given, unmasking mutations otherwise unrecognizable, is not a good
reason for inbreeding. If a genetic disease is uncovered by inbreeding,
the breeder would need to eliminate both lines used for this breeding
because the heterozygotic animals can not be detected on phenotype
alone. The argument of testbreeding to a known carrier sounds good
on paper but would require excessive culling of puppies and, minimally,
sterilization of the tested lines which are also needed to generate
affected dogs for the next generation of test matings. It seems,
the very genetic techniques that will eventually allow us to correct
these mutations will also allow us to screen for mutations without
going through the peril of inbreeding with its highly unlikely cure
of purging all deleterious genes. Thus, in the next millennium we
will probably be able to debunk the inbreeding myth simply by showing
that its single alleged application, testbreeding and purging, is
not a rational way to handle genetic problems in a breed. After
all, wolves are conforming to their type based on a high genetic
variation (Vila et al., 1997). The challenge will be to generate
type in combination with genetic variation rather than depleting
this by excessive inbreeding.
Literature: Lin, L., Faraco, J., Li, R., Kadotani, H., Rogers,
W., Lin, X, Qiu, X., de Jong, P.J., Nishino, S., and Mignot, E.,
(1999) The sleep disorder canine narcolepsy is caused by a mutation
in the Hyporetin (Orexin) receptor 2 gene. Cell 98: 365-376. Page,
R.D.M. and Holmes, E.C. (1998) Molecular Evolution: a phylogenetic
approach. Blackwell Science, pp. 346 Pennisi, E. (1999) The perils
of genetic purging. Science 285: 193. Frankham, R. and Ralls, K.
(1998) Inbreeding leads to extinction. Nature 392: 441-441. Stachowicz,
J.J., Whitlach, R.B., and Osman, R.W. (1999) Species diversity and
invasion resistance in a marine ecosystem. Science 286: 1577-1579.
Stone, S.L., Arnoldo, M.A., and Goring, D.R. (1999) A breakdown
of Brassica self-incompatibility in ARC1 antisense transgenic plants.
Science 286: 1729-1731. Vila, C., Savolainen, P., Maldonado, J.E.,
Arnorim, I.R., Rice, J.E., Honeycutt, R.L., Crandall, K.A., Lundeberg,
J., and Wayne, R.K. (1997) Multiple and ancient origins of the domestic
dog. Science 276: 1687-1689.
|